Many people, even today, like to
see humans as something apart from the rest of the world—the special species
that either has so far outdistanced our nearest relatives that comparisons are
no longer useful or even possible; or the special
species created by God in his own image and likeness, and therefore not
even related to the rest of brute creation. Either view massages the human
sense of ourselves as elevated, different, and with ultimate dominion over all
our planetary co-habitants. Made by God or Nature to rule, we can do whatever
we wish to not just the domestic animals we raise and manipulate for food, but
to all wild animals as well. And what particularly gives us this sense of
ourselves as unique and uniquely in charge is our morality. Though our bodies
may run on the same kinds of energy and operate with the same kinds of cells
and bodily structures and even brains common to most life forms, and though
some primates may be able to communicate using signs we’ve taught them, no
other animal has an even remotely comparable sense of fairness, of compassion,
of justice. And all those abstract qualities depend chiefly on our highly
developed sense of reason—on our brain-centered rationality. We alone can look
at a problem, figure out its origins and causes, and come up with a rational
solution.
Now
anyone who has been paying attention to science in recent years knows that such
views have been getting a terrible buffeting since the days of Charles Darwin.
Far from being separate, Darwin showed what subsequent scientists have filled
out and firmed up: we descend directly from the rest of creation, and our
distance from our nearest relatives, the great apes, seems ever more narrow. We
share at least 98.7% of our DNA with bonobos and chimpanzees. Which, again,
might be acceptable and leave us some “special” room if only we could still
claim some distinction like morality. Alas, according to Frans deWaal, a
researcher at Emory University’s renowned primate lab and author of The Bonobo and the Atheist: In Search of
Humanism Among the Primates (Norton: 2013), this small comfort can no
longer be maintained. According to deWaal, and several others he cites, recent
research using neuroscience demonstrates that most of our vaunted moral and social
distinctions can be found in similar form among bonobos or chimps, or both.
Their roots can even be found among just about all mammals as well. What this
means is that far from being a gift from the God we’ve been worshipping for
several thousands of years in a variety of religions (that great theologian
Ronald Reagan in 1984 said: “as
morality’s foundation is religion, religion and politics are necessarily
related…”), morality is actually built into our brains, especially our
emotional brains.
This
seems to me to be good news. It explains that unlike many conservatives and
religionists, who see humans as basically ‘bad’ and therefore in need of harsh
rules and punitive controls to ‘civilize’ us, we humans, with our basic
equipment deriving from apes, are programmed not only to be good but also to do
good. deWaal quotes his colleague at Emory, James Rilling, to the effect that
we have “emotional biases toward cooperation that can only be overcome with
effortful cognitive control” (49). As deWaal explains it, this means “that our
first impulse is to trust and assist; only secondarily do we weigh the option
of not doing so, for which we need
reasons.” Using brain-monitoring techniques, Rilling showed that when normal
people aid others, “brain areas associated with reward are activated.” As
deWaal succinctly puts it: Doing good
feels good. To be sure, there are psychopaths in any population whose
brains, for whatever reasons, lack these emotional rewards. But in a later
section, deWaal follows Chris Boehm in surmising that evolution has probably
worked to marginalize these outliers: the penalty, in social groups, for not
cooperating can be harsh, ranging from ostracism to complete elimination—thus minimizing
the propagation of those with such genes.
What
deWaal does in the rest of his book is to show that far from being imposed from
above by a law-giving God, or from reasoning by deep-thinking philosophers, the
moral law in fact “arises from ingrained values that have been there since the
beginning of time” (228). This is especially true among animals who live and
hunt cooperatively, like dogs, chimpanzees and bonobos:
The
most fundamental one [i.e. value] derives from the survival value of group
life. The desire to belong, to get along, to love and be loved, prompts us to
do everything in our power to stay on good terms with those on whom we depend.
Other social primates share this value and rely on the same filter between
emotion and action [i.e. on inhibitions] to reach a mutually agreeable modus
vivendi. (228).
It also applies to our Neanderthal
relatives, who, recent fossil evidence shows, took care of the infirm
(individuals afflicted with dwarfism, paralysis, or the inability to chew
survived into adulthood) in the same way early humans did. Since Neanderthals
lived hundreds of millennia before civilization and its gods, this means, again,
that morality and its basis in empathy existed well before civilization itself.
Further, since the evidence of empathy among primates is by now
well-established (mirror neurons, the brain cells that mediate empathy and
allow us to feel what another is going through, were first discovered in
macaque monkeys), it seems quite clear that humans easily adopted the moral
laws promoted by religions (like the ten commandments) primarily because they
were already inclined to be moral. To cooperate. To help others in order that
more of the group might survive. And, from the negative side, to avoid hurting
others by inhibiting the impulse to do them damage. As deWaal sums it up, “a
social hierarchy is a giant system of inhibitions, which is no doubt what paved
the way for human morality, which is also such a system” (150).
This
latter point has great relevance for our time, it seems to me. For we are even
now engaged in a great debate about what economic system best fits our human
nature. Up till very recently, we have been told that capitalism is “natural,”
that competition is “natural,” that the war of every individual against every
other individual to monopolize resources is “natural.” But if our primate
inheritance prizes cooperation, prizes helping others, inhibits us from hurting
others, puts the welfare of the group or community above the individual impulse
to harm or to hog everything to oneself, then this attribution of “natural” would
seem to have serious shortcomings. Consider what deWaal writes about the ideas
of fairness and justice, both fundamental components of any moral law. He first
cites the “egalitarianism” of hunter-gatherer groups, where “hunters aren’t
even allowed to carve up their own kill, in order to prevent them from favoring
family and friends” (231). In other words, the inhibition against taking all
for oneself or one’s family is a primary form of fairness—and ultimately, of
course, a way to ensure not only that all members of the group get a share in
any one individual’s luck, but also that reciprocity will dictate that the same
fair division will happen when another individual brings home a kill. Many
experiments have shown how ingrained this preference or insistence on an even split is, not only among humans, but also
among our primate relatives. In one experiment, capuchin monkeys were playing a
game, the reward for which was cucumber slices. All the monkeys were ok with
this, and played the game. But when the experimenters started to reward some of the monkeys with grapes (a
preferred food), the ones still given cucumbers vehemently protested, and
indeed, refused to accept the cucumbers at all. They actually tried to destroy
the whole game. Economists would call this refusal of perfectly good food
“irrational;” but, as deWaal points out, “it is an irrationality that
transcends species.” It is a deeply emotional fairness response that all
primates exhibit, and that even dogs (also group hunters) exhibit as well.
deWaal cites a finding by Friederike Range at the University of Vienna where
“dogs refuse to lift their paw for a ‘shake’ with a human if they get nothing
for it while a companion dog is rewarded” (234). deWaal summarizes these
findings about fairness and justice as “ancient capacities,” which “derive from
the need to preserve harmony in the face of resource competition” (234).
Preserving
harmony. The primacy of group welfare. Cooperation. Anger and refusal in the
face of inequity. Suddenly it seems that much of the discontent coursing
through modern societies is not derived from some outlandish and artificial
notion of social justice and fair play. Suddenly it seems that humans, like all
other primates, are primed to react emotionally—however irrational it may seem
to some—to perceived unfairness and the unequal distribution of goods. When
Occupy Wall Street protesters shout and protest about the 1 percent taking all
the wealth, leaving the 99 percent with crumbs; when Bernie Sanders, running
for president, rages about the obscenely unequal distribution of wealth in the
United States; when fast-food workers demand a living wage in spite of their
low-skilled jobs; when Greeks demand that their government refuse to pay off loans
to predatory banks while cutting pensions and health care; when we all react
with nausea when we read of corporate heads paying less in taxes than their
secretaries; we should begin to see this not as illogical or irrational but as
an upwelling from an ancient part of the primate brain that is built in to what
it means to be human. And the constant harping on individuality and “looking
out for number one”? That we should see as a regrettable leftover from outlier
impulses that should have been, and should still be relegated to the genetic waste
bin. Morals and the moral code itself derive from our deep inheritance as
cooperating animals whose primary impulse is to get along in order to survive;
to inhibit self-centered accumulation as destructive of group harmony; and to
help others because helping others feels good.
Only
then, after we have caught up with our primate kin, might we be ready to
fulfill our role as fully-human human beings. Which is to say, going even
beyond the primate need for in-group survival and realizing, as only humans
can, that we must share our empathy and our protection to all life forms that
share this planet with us, that literally make it possible for us to even be
here.
Lawrence DiStasi
No comments:
Post a Comment